As political pressure builds for improving completion rates
of community college students, we teachers find that our practices,
organizational and pedagogical, require justification. The assumption “out there” seems to be that
research will uncover the appropriate institutional structure and teaching
methods. Looking for a silver bullet is
paying a lot of peoples’ mortgages, but the findings, on examination, are so
tentative (or should be) as to offer little guidance.
The news from the Department of Education’s Institute for
Education Sciences in 2013 that only “12% of the [90] interventions produced
positive effects” should be a heads-up that all is not well for our intentions
to practice “research-based” instruction.
Results are weakly correlated to practices because of poor experimental
design or execution in part, but also because live social science
experimentation is inherently messy.
Controlling for experimenter bias is also a concern, as Diane Ravitch
documents in The Death and Life of the
Great American School System (128-145).
And then there’s the issue of publication bias, the tendency of journals
to publish positive results rather than failures to confirm hypotheses.
Some school districts have been hyping neuroscience as the
truly reliable source of scientific information on how we learn and
remember. Beginning with a look at brain
geography and function, the in-service training typically demonstrates, through
visuals of various brain images, the centers of various functions such as short
and long-term memory, language, logical processing, and so on. Changes in neural structures are described as
the physical manifestation of “learning.”
Even a cheerleader like Eric Jensen, in his Teaching with the Brain in Mind,
concedes early that “…educators can apply only a small percentage of brain
research” (5). Why that might be true is
perhaps suggested by excerpts from The
Learning Brain: Lessons for Education
by neuroscientists Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, and Uta Frith:
So
what is happening in the brain when we learn something at this unconscious,
implicit, level? Using positron emission tomography (PET),
Jonathan Cohen and colleagues at the University of Pittsburgh mapped the brain regions
that are responsive to implicit learning of sequences. Volunteers performed the task (of tacit rule
building when shown sequences of letters).
They had no idea of learning anything.
Once
the participants were trained, they were scanned. When there was a subtle change in the nature
of the sequence, this resulted in blood flow increases in a network of brain
regions including the left premotor area and
anterior cingulate, and part of the basal ganglia on the right. Blood flow decreases at the rule break were
observed in the right prefrontal cortex. These changes suggest that these regions are
responsive to the rule break, which can occur without awareness. The brain notices things that you do not
(142).
Now I think it’s interesting that our brains indeed “think”
without our awareness of it, but I don’t know how else our normal speech would
otherwise be possible, as we don’t generally formulate our thoughts in words
consciously unless we are being very careful, and even then our access to our
lexicon is not an act we can monitor.
The words just come—or not. I don't need a neuroscientist to tell me that it happens.
And what increasing blood flow in an area of the brain can tell
me about how I should organize Freshman Comp or teach comma use is an open
question.
Another comment from our British brain scientists:
It is unlikely that there is one
single all-purpose type of learning for everything. In terms of brain structures involved,
learning mathematics differs from learning to read, which differs from learning
to play the piano. Each memory system
relies on a different brain system and develops at a slightly different
time. Remembering who you are differs
from remembering where you are (139).
I have argued this point in earlier posts without the aid of
neuroscience, so this can’t be considered particularly insightful. If we are to accept Chomsky’s contention of
the innateness of language structures, that doesn’t preclude our ability to
generalize from instances (empiricism) or develop knowledge that language can
not adequately describe (Polanyi’s “personal” knowledge). This should surprise no one. It makes sense that our ancient ancestors
would process any and all available experience and sensory inputs to survive
and thrive to the best of their ability.
They didn’t debate epistemology, they just learned whatever they could
in whatever way they could or had to.
The invention of writing simply gave them new sources of packaging
knowledge and learning it anew.
Even Blakemore and Frith seem to acknowledge that the era of
neuroscience-inspired pedagogy is a ways off:
“Based on findings from neuroscience, we can imagine a day when we will
be able to use all sorts of radical new ways to improve learning and memory”
(17).
There may be a problem, however, with this prognosis. William Uttal, Professor Emeritus in
Psychology at the University of Michigan, challenges any notion of mapping what
the brain does to what we know ourselves to be doing and learning:
The empirical facts
we encounter…should raise our awareness of the actual complexity of the mind-brain
system. As such, they emphasize the
increasing difficulty and ambiguity of the scientific situation rather than
clarify the nature of the mind-brain relation.
The brain probably should not now be considered to be an aggregate of
isolated or isolatable components located in a particular place but, rather, as
a dynamically changing four-dimensional (x,
y, z, t) network of interconnecting and interacting components of vaguely
defined, redundant, and overlapping functions and, thus, ever-changing spatial
limits. No longer is the crutch for simplifying
research designs offered by the neophrenological idea of the localized
representation of cognitive faculties (either singly or as separate components
of a “system”) available to us as a means of simplifying and organizing our
findings. It is now increasingly
apparent that brain images at the very least reflect the fact that vast regions
of the brain, if not all of it, are involved in the simplest cognitive
processes (45).
He concludes pessimistically:
What
all this means is that the MRI and the EEG are blunt instruments—epistemological
sledge hammers—when it comes to understanding or even representing the detailed
neuronal network mechanisms that actually underlie cognitive processes. They are techniques that operate at the wrong
level of analysis; where we need information about the patterns of microscopic
neuronal activity to understand something like learning, we only have available
measures that pool all of the truly salient microscopic information into an unanalyzable
compound. It may not be too severe a
criticism to point out that whatever signs or biomarkers of cognitive activity
may ultimately emerge from brain imaging studies, the whole enterprise is
theoretically sterile because of this disconnect between the level at which we
observe (molar chunks of the brain) and the level at which mind is actually
instantiated (the details of the network).
A logical conclusion of this argument is that any hope for practical
applications that assume that we will be able to “read the mind” or use these
tools as “biomarkers” for either normal or dysfunctional mental processes is
unlikely to be realized (46).
How the brain actually works and what we can see are
incommensurable. Furthermore, the
knowledge of what the brain does and the knowledge of what we do and how we learn
are fundamentally and inherently non-translatable
one to the other: “there is no direct
access to the conscious experiences that permit us to directly compare mental and
neural events” (8).
Time to re-boot.
Resources
Blakemore,
Sarah-Jayne and Uta Frith. The Learning Brain: Lessons for Education. Malden, MA: Blackwell, 2005. Print.
Jensen,
Eric. Teaching with the Brain in Mind.
Alexandria, VA: ASCD, 1998. Print.
Ravitch, Diane. The Death and Life of the Great American School System: How Testing and Choice are undermining Education. New York: Basic Books, 2010.
Ravitch, Diane. The Death and Life of the Great American School System: How Testing and Choice are undermining Education. New York: Basic Books, 2010.
Uttal, William
R. Mind
and Brain: A Critical Appraisal of
Cognitive Neuroscience. Cambridge,
MA: MIT, 2011. Print.
No comments:
Post a Comment